... but so much time is spent on searching, reading, talking (online), when is there time to work? So I put the work first. Some day I will write more (or someone will write about me ;-)
Sunday, September 30, 2012
Saturday, September 15, 2012
Art illiteracy
When was the last time you went to a museum, bought an art book or stopped for 10 minutes to look at a work of art you came across online? Probably you are missing something from the beauty of life. It can be taught, going deeper in an image and taking out the juice of joy, of being alive and deciding for yourself what makes you grow (as opposed to doing what everyone else does, because it’s trendy).
But most people hate learning and we have the parents and the schools to thank for that.
Most people “read” an image at the surface, this is how we got to the point of valuing the “new and shocking”, this is why “artists” such as Demien Hirst, Tracy Emin, etc exist: they “challenge your brains” and move art from what art is good for to snobbing you with “I thought of this first!”.
We all look at a van Gogh, we all see a different thing, we all understand a different thing. That understanding of art is not done with the brain. That special understanding is made in a state of being which, if not developed and kept alive, is drying out. And so, we become beings chasing something we think we know, to fill a hole in ourselves that we think we understand.
Saturday, September 1, 2012
Draw, don't trace
Drawing Ability Has Psychological Basis In
Perception And Memory, Researchers Say
By: Natalie Wolchover
Published: 04/24/2012 05:57 PM EDT on Lifes Little Mysteries
Published: 04/24/2012 05:57 PM EDT on Lifes Little Mysteries
Since the dawn of human art-making, the divide has been clear:
There are people who can effortlessly sketch an object's likeness, and people
who struggle for hours just to get the angles and proportions right (by which
point the picture is scarred by eraser marks, anyway). What separates the
drawers from the drawer-nots?
Ongoing research is revealing the answer to this longstanding
question. It seems that realistic drawing ability hinges on three factors: how
a person perceives reality, how well he or she remembers visual information
from one moment to the next, and which elements of an object he or she selects
to actually draw.
If you're stuck on stick figures, the good news, according to
researchers at the University College London, is that people can improve at all
these mental processes with practice.
First,
people who can't draw well aren't seeing the world as it really is. When we
look at an object, our visual systems automatically misjudge such attributes as
size, shape and color; research over the past three years shows at least some
of these misperceptions translate into drawing errors. Paradoxically, in other
circumstances the misperceptions help us make sense of the world. For example,
objects appear larger when they are closer than when they are far away. Even
so, the visual system practices "size constancy" by perceiving the
object as being approximately one size no matter how far away it is. The visual
system, "knowing" a distant object is really bigger than it appears, sends false information to the brain about what the eyeball is seeing.
People
who have the most trouble judging apparent size, shape, color and brightness
may also be the worst at drawing, recent research by Justin Ostrofsky and his
colleagues at Brooklyn College and the Graduate Center of the City University
of New York suggests. Those who draw well are better able to override these
visual misperceptions and perceive what their own eyeballs are really seeing. [Red-Green & Blue-Yellow: The Stunning Colors You Can't See]
However,
inaccurately perceiving the image is only part of the story, said Rebecca Chamberlain,
a psychologist at University College London. Chamberlain and her colleagues
recently conducted experiments investigating the role of visual memory in the
drawing process. They believe that drawing skill results in part from an
ability to remember simple relationships in an object ? such as an angle
between two lines ? from the moment the angle is perceived to the moment it is
drawn. Additionally, "drawing seems to involve focusing on both holistic
proportional relationships as well as focus on detail isolated from the whole.
Perhaps it is the ability to switch between these two modes of seeing that
underpins successful drawing," Chamberlain told us.
Furthermore, as detailed in December in the journal Psychology of Aesthetics,
Creativity, and the Arts, Ostrofsky and his colleagues found significant
evidence that skilled artists are better at selecting which elements of an
object need to be included to convey the object's form. And once the artists
have selected an important element, they are better at focusing their attention
on it and ignoring extraneous details nearby.
The
devil is in the details, and the researchers are still working out the
interplay between all the factors that affect drawing accuracy. However, they
can all be learned. "There is no doubt that practice is an important
component of being able to draw," Chamberlain said. While some may be
predisposed to be better at perceptual accuracy and visual memory than others,
"the rest of us use tricks to emulate this."
In research presented at a recent symposium at Columbia
University and soon to be published by Columbia University Press, Chamberlain
and her colleagues found practicing drawing significantly improved people's
abilities over time, as rated by other people who participated in the study.
Based on their research, the psychologists recommended the
following techniques for getting better at drawing: Focus on scaling a drawing
to fit the size of the paper; anchor an object in its surroundings by showing
how it sits in space; focus on the distance between elements of the object and
on their relative sizes; and focus on the size and shape of "negative
space," or the empty space between parts of the object. Lastly, they
recommend thinking of "lines" as what they really are -- boundaries
between light and dark areas.
As Chris McManus, a member of the research team, noted,
"There are few human skills which don't improve with practice."
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)